Asian Journal of Physics Vol 32, Nos 3 & 4 (2023) 159-166

Microsaccadic eye movement orientations are equivocal in the presence of competing stimuli

Eswar Kurni1, Manish Reddy Yedulla2 and PremNandhini Satgunam1
1Brien Holden Institute of Optometry and Vision Sciences, Hyderabad Eye Research Foundation,
L V Prasad Eye Institute, Hyderabad- 500 034, India
2Department of Engineering Science, Indian Institute of Technology, Hyderabad-502 285, India
Dedicated to Prof Jay M Enoch


Will someone reflexively look towards a primed target or to a non-primed target, when no instructions are given? Knowing this could help design visual function tests without the need for instructions. Simply, a target could be presented for a “priming phase” followed by two targets one of which is the primed target and the other is not. We asked the question to which target will an obsever look. We studied this on normally-sighted adults. Eye movements were tracked using EyeLink1000 Plus eye tracker and microsaccades were analyzed. The targets presented were from LEA symbols that are commonly used in children’s visual acuity chart. Target size (15′, 20′ or 25′) and presentation duration (200, 400 or 600 ms) were randomized. No instructions were given to the participants beyond asking them to look at the computer monitor in experiment I, and instructions were given to specifically look towards the primed target in experiment II. Overall we found that no preference (proportion of microsaccades <50%) was observed either to the primed or to the novel target in either of the experiments. The presence of two competing stimuli abolishes the microsaccde orientation to a target of interest, even with explicit verbal instructions. © Anita Publications. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Microsaccades, Preferential looking, Visual search, Latency, Priming, Eye movements.


Peer Review Information
Method: Single- anonymous; Screened for Plagiarism? Yes
Buy this Article in Print © Anita Publications. All rights reserve

References

  1. Enoch J M, A rapid, accurate technique for retinoscopy of the aphakic infant or child in the operating room, Am J Ophthalmol, 78(1974)335–336.
  2. Johansen A, White S, Waraisch P, Screening for visual impairment in older people: validation of the Cardiff Acuity Test, Arch Gerontol Geriatr, 36(2003)289–293.
  3. Crabb D P, Smith N D, Zhu H, What’s on TV? Detecting age-related neurodegenerative eye disease using eye movement scanpaths, Front Aging Neurosci, 6(2014); doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00312.
  4. Hayhoe M M, Shrivastava A, Mruczek R, Pelz J B,Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task, J Vis, 3(2003)49–63.
  5. Houston-Price C, Nakai C S, Distinguishing novelty and familiarity effects in infant preference procedures, Inf Child Dev, 13(2004)341–348.
  6. Heise N, Ansorge U, The roles of scene priming and location priming in object-scene consistency effects. Front Psychol, 5(2014)520; doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00520.
  7. Engbert R, Kliegl R, Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention, Vis Res, 43(2003)1035–1045.
  8. Hafed Z M, Clark J J, Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts, Vis Res, 42(2002)2533–2545.
  9. Hyvarinen L, Nasanen R, Laurinen P, New visual acuity test for pre-school children, Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh), 58(1980)507–511.
  10. Brainard D H, The Psychophysics Toolbox, Spat Vis, 10(1997)433–436.
  11. Pelli D G, The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies, Spat Vis, 10(1997)437–442.
  12. Takagi M, Frohman E M, Zee D S, Gap-overlap effects on latencies of saccades, vergence and combined vergence-saccades in humans, Vis Res, 35(1995)3373–3388.
  13. Dandona L, Dandona R, Revision of visual impairment definitions in the International Statistical Classification of Diseases, BMC Med, 4(2006)7; doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-4-7.
  14. Biederman I, Cooper E E, Size invariance in visual object priming, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 18(1992)121–133.
  15. Cornelissen F W, Peters E M, Palmer J, The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox, Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 34(2002)613–617.
  16. Engbert R, Mergenthaler K, Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip, Proc Natl Acad Sci (U S A), 103(2006)7192–7197.
  17. Otero-Millan J, Troncoso X G, Macknik S L, Serrano-Pedraza I; Susana Martinez-Conde S, Saccades and microsaccades during visual fixation, exploration, and search: foundations for a common saccadic generator, J Vis, 8(2008); doi.org/10.1167/8.14.21.
  18. Rolfs M, Microsaccades: small steps on a long way, Vis Res, 49(2009)2415–2441.
  19. Yarbus A, Eye movements and vision, New York, 1967. Plenum Press, New York),1967; [Erste russische Ausgabe, 1965]. Dipl.-Inf. Marc Pomplun.
  20. Brascamp J W, Blake R, Kristjansson A, Deciding where to attend: priming of pop-out drives target selection, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 37(2011)1700–1707.
  21. Rock I, Linnett C M, Grant P, Mack A, Perception without attention: results of a new method, Cogn Psychol, 24(1992)502–534.
  22. Theeuwes J, Kramer A F, Hahn S, Irwin D E, Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects, Psychological Science, 9(1998)379–385.
  23. Jolij J, Scholte H S, van Gaal S, Hodgson T L, Lamme V A F, Act quickly, decide later: long-latency visual processing underlies perceptual decisions but not reflexive behavior, J Cogn Neurosci, 23(2011)3734–3745.
  24. Yuval-Greenberg S, Merriam E P, Heeger D J, Spontaneous microsaccades reflect shifts in covert attention, J Neurosci, 34(2014)13693–13700.
  25. Ko H K, Poletti M, Rucci M, Microsaccades precisely relocate gaze in a high visual acuity task, Nat Neurosci, 13(2010)1549–1553.